Déjà Vu

views updated Jun 08 2018

DÉJÀ VU

Déjà vu refers to a state wherein a person feels certain (cognitive judgment) that he or she has previously seen or experienced something that is actually being encountered for the first time. Sigmund Freud believed the feeling corresponded to the memory of an unconscious daydream.

The term first appeared in a French translation of the Psychopathology of Everyday Life (1901b) as part of the discussion of the superstition that can be associated with this mysterious feeling. Freud quotes certain "psychologists," without specifying who they are. The concept falls squarely within the framework of the paramnesia extensively described by psychiatrists in France, primarily Wigan (1844) and Valentin Magnan (1893), who described systematic delirium accompanied by the illusion of doppelgängers, J. Capgras (1923), who described the illusion of doppelgangers, and Pierre Janet (1905), who described cases of false recognition.

Freud discusses the concept in terms of the psycho-pathology of everyday life (errors, slips) by removing it from the context of psychosis and by supporting it with his own self-analysis ("rapid sensations of déjà vu that I myself experienced"). He returned to it again, but within the context of therapy, in his "Fausse reconnaissance (déjà raconté ) in Psycho-Analytic Treatment" (1914a), referring to a central example of the analysis of the Wolf Man. He then provided a partial summary of authors who had discussed the issue, separating them into "believers" (who thought that déjà vu was proof of a previous existence), among whom he includes Pythagoras, and "nonbelievers," who regard such events as false memories (Wigan, 1860). Freud himself assumes a different position (which he acknowledges sharing with Joseph Grasset, 1904) by believing in the reality of the representative content, but associating this with the reactivation of an older unconscious impression. He returned to the question again in terms of self-analysis at the end of his life in "A Disturbance of Memory on the Acropolis" (1936a).

Déjà vu is one of the "uncanny feelings" that, for Freud, play the role of hallucinations, which become more frequent and systematic during certain mental disturbances. This is the most convincing example of breaching the boundary between the normal and the pathological addressed by Freud. It involves a dissociative type of change experienced by the subject in his or her perception of things or himself. Reality appears distant, like a dream or a shadow, and it is at this point that false recognition occurs. Along with this displacement of the perceived object from the present into the past, there is a confused feeling of expectation or foreknowledge, whereby the subject is simultaneously projected into the future. For Freud this involves the replacement of some part of reality by a repressed desire (1901b). In the example cited here, a young girl replaces the perception of her wish to have seen her brother die with the sensation of having already experienced the situation (a trip to the countryside to visit some young girls whose brother is seriously ill). The topographic displacement (unconscious/conscious) is also spatio-temporal, for the memory involves the house and the girls' dresses but not the brother's illness. In "A Disturbance of Memory on the Acropolis," the same phenomenon is reversed since the reality of the Acropolis dissolves within the feeling of disbelief Freud experiences. Here, doubt replaces certainty; doubt is awakened by the reality of the perception but contaminates perception at the same time.

The concept of déjà vu must be compared with other analogous terms in analysis, such as déjà vécu (already experienced) and déjà raconté (already communicated). According to Freud, this paramnesia can be explained as a confusion between the intention to communicate and its realization. As with the doubt in his dream, these forms of paramnesia refer to specifically significant facts, such as the hallucination of the severed finger that the Wolf Man is convinced he has already told Freud about, when, in fact, he had only mentioned the existence of the small knife carried by his uncle. Generally speaking, paramnesia leads to a reflection on the process of remembering during therapy and on the patient's illusion of having "always known" the repressed content revealed by interpretation ("Remembering, Repeating, Working-through"). "It is by this means," Freud writes, "that the problem of analysis is resolved" (1914g).

Déjà vu touches on the whole question of forgetting as a dissociation of memory, as well as on the question of true and false from the psychoanalytic point of view. The false recognition of Norbert Harnold ("Is it a 'real' ghost?") is the true recognition of the originally invested object displaced within the context of archeology in Delusions and Dreams in Jensen's "Gradiva" (1907a [1906j]).

Sophie de Mijolla-Mellor

See also: Certainty; Estrangement; Illusion.

Bibliography

Freud, Sigmund. (1901b). The psychopathology of everyday life. SE,6.

. (1907a [1906j]). Delusions and dreams in Jensen's "Gradiva." SE, 9: 1-95.

. (1914a). Fausse reconnaissance ("déjà raconté") in psycho-analytic treatment. SE, 13: 199-207.

. (1914g). Remembering, repeating, working-through (Further recommendations on the technique of psycho-analysis II). SE, 12: 147-156.

. (1936a). A disturbance of memory on the Acropolis (an open letter to Romain Rolland on the occasion of his seventieth birthday). SE, 22, 239-248.

Déjà Vu

views updated May 23 2018

DÉJÀ VU

Déjà vu is a memory phenomenon widely experienced by the general public and is often cited in the popular literature. While the experience was described in the scientific literature as early as the mid-1800s, researchers used a variety of different terms to describe the experience (e.g., paramnesia) until the middle of the twentieth century. A standard definition of déjà vu used today was provided by Neppe (1983, p. 3): "any subjectively inappropriate impression of familiarity of a present experience with an undefined past."

Survey research indicates that between half to two-thirds of individuals have had a déjà vu experience, although this estimate varies considerably (30 to 97 percent) across three dozen investigations. Of those who experience déjà vu, nearly all have had more than one, and most experience one or more each year. A déjà vu experience is typically triggered by a visual scene, lasts for a few seconds, and is associated with mild stress, anxiety, or fatigue. The primary psychological reaction is surprise, and the time sense seems to slow down.

The incidence of déjà vu decreases systematically with age, and this is probably due to an increase in societal awareness across recent years. For example, Gallup and Newport (1991) found that belief in déjà vu nearly doubled between 1978 and 1990. There may be a decrease in reports of déjà vu with age because older adults grew up in a time when belief in déjà vu was not as widely accepted as it is today.

Déjà vu appears to be more common in better-educated and better-traveled individuals; there is no evidence of gender differences. Déjà vu also occurs in the aura preceding a seizure in some temporal lobe epileptics (TLEs). This trend motivated research to determine whether déjà vu is a potential diagnostic tool in seizure activity, epilepsy, and brain pathology. Accumulating evidence, however, does not support such speculation.

Explanations of Déjà Vu

Interpretations of déjà have varied from the parapsychological (a telepathic reversion to a previous lifetime) to the psychodynamic (the mind neutralizes an emotional situation by displacing it into the past). These theses are thoroughly discussed by Sno and Linszen (1990) and Neppe (1983). We will examine several possible scientific interpretations.

Neurological Explanations

For more than a century, researchers have suggested that a déjà vu experience reflects a neurological dysfunction. Since déjà vu is part of the preseizure aura in some TLEs, it seems reasonable that déjà vu in nonepileptics may result from small temporal lobe seizures. Another interpretation is that the déjà vu experience results from a momentary delay in neuronal transmission from the perceptual organ to the higher processing centers of the brain. This slight (several millisecond) increase in the normal time taken to transmit the message because of a synaptic dysfunction may lead to a misinterpretation of the information as being old.

Another neurological interpretation involves two pathways rather than one. In the visual system, information is received in the cortex first from the primary and then a secondary pathway. When the normally brief delay between the two tracks lengthens, the usually seamless integration of the two messages is disrupted and experienced as two separate messages. A variation on this position is that the primary rather than the secondary neuronal path is inordinately slowed, resulting in a reversal of the normal sequence of messages. We routinely interpret information from the primary pathway as our initial perception, so when the secondary pathway arrives slightly before the primary pathway, the information from the primary pathway feels familiar because a "memory" match already exists—it was established moments before.

Memory Explanations

It is possible that the individual experienced the present situation not directly but through a magazine, movie, TV show, or dream. Considerable research suggests that source confusions are routine and that déjà vu may reflect a match with a "memory" created by media or imagination. Another memory perspective is that the type of cognitive processing is being duplicated rather than the actual memory. Retrieval success often depends upon the correspondence between the way information is processed during input and retrieval. If the cognitive processing of new information follows a set of mental procedures similar to those of a prior experience, an unexpected sense of familiarity may result.

Another memory perspective is that one aspect of the present setting is objectively familiar, but conscious recognition fails when the object appears in a different context. The familiarity elicited by the unidentified object is then overgeneralized to the entire setting. On a visit to a friend's home for the first time, for example, the grandfather clock in the corner is identical to one in your aunt's home. The implicit familiarity of this object is not connected to the "old" object but instead misattributed to the objectively new setting.

Finally, the present setting may evoke a sense of déjà vu because of a familiar organization of the elements in a scene. It is not the grandfather clock in the living room of your friend's new home that is familiar but rather that the room's layout: a sofa to the right of the love seat, with a stairway to the left and a grandfather clock against the back wall, the same as in your aunt's house.

Attentional Explanations

An ongoing perceptual experience may occasionally be divided into separate perceptions through distraction or inattention. In this explanation, first proposed by Titchener in 1924, a brief initial perception under diminished attention is followed immediately by a second perception under full attention. The second impression matches the "ignored" first glance taken moments earlier, giving rise to the feeling that the current experience duplicates something experienced before. As you walk into a new hotel lobby talking on your cell phone, you process the scene without full attention. When you hang up, you perceive the situation with full awareness and get the feeling that you have been here before. Recent research on inattentional blindness by Mack and Rock (1998) suggests that individuals often miss clearly visible objects if they are focused on something else, even when the unattended object is directly in front of them.

Dual-Processing Explanations

Routine cognitive experience may often involve the operation of two separate but interactive processes. While both processes usually operate in concert, occasionally they might shift out of synchrony, or one process might occur in the absence of the other. For example, retrieval and familiarity usually operate in an independent but coordinated manner, with recall accompanied by familiarity. When retrieval is activated without familiarity, a familiar setting seems momentarily unfamiliar (jamais vu). Conversely, when familiarity is activated in the absence of retrieval, déjà vu is the result. Another interpretation is that memory encoding and retrieval usually operate independently of each other. We experience a new situation; then we encode it into memory. However, if both encoding and retrieval are simultaneously activated, this spurious familiarity results in déjà vu.

Finally, we may have two different varieties of consciousness: One processes information from the environment, whereas the other monitors our inner, mental world. Déjà vu may occur when normal consciousness is diminished by distraction, fatigue, or seizure, forcing reliance on the internal consciousness operating from internally generated images, resulting in a misreading of a new experience as old.

In summary, déjà vu is experienced by most people on an average of once per year. Its incidence decreases with age, increases with education, and is more common under stress or fatigue. Likely explanations include neurological dysfunction (seizure, synaptic slowdown), implicit familiarity of objects, divided attention followed by full perception, and/or alteration in the normal function of two cognitive processes.

Bibliography

Gallup, G. H., and Newport, F. (1991). Belief in paranormal phenomena among adult Americans. The Skeptical Inquirer 15, 137-146.

Mack, A., and Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Neppe, V. M. (1983). The psychology of déjà vu: Have I been here before? Johannesburg: Witwatersrand University Press.

Sno, H. N., and Linszen, D. H. (1990). The déjà vu experience: Remembrance of things past? American Journal of Psychiatry 147, 1,587-1,595.

Titchener, E. B. (1924). A beginner's psychology. New York: Macmillan

Alan S.Brown

Déjà Vu

views updated May 29 2018

Déjà Vu

A French term used by psychical researchers to characterize the feeling people sometimes have that some scene or experience in the present also occurred in the past. Déjà vu (already seen) is often coupled with déjà entendu (already heard). Through the years, many have related the feelings of déjà vu to the phenomenon of astral projection or out-of-the-body travel, when individuals apparently visit a distant place in an astral or etheric body during sleep. Déjà vu is also associated with fulfillment of a prior premonition of a forthcoming event.

More recently, déjà vu has been connected to experiences of reincarnation, when a feeling of prior knowledge is so strong that people feel sure it must have come from a former incarnation. In a celebrated case in India, a little girl named Shanti Devi, born in Delhi in 1926, claimed that she had lived elsewhere in a former birth, and even named the city. When taken there, she correctly identified the house, family, and other circumstantial details.

Feelings of déjà vu are rarely evidential or even reliable. Scenes in the present may only appear familiar because they contain some element connected with a past experience and re-activate the sensation of familiarity. Psychologists have characterized the phenomenon of false remembering as "postidentifying paramnesia."

Sources:

Berger, Arthur S., and Joyce Berger. The Encyclopedia of Parapsychology and Psychical Research. New York: Paragon House, 1991.

déjà vu

views updated May 18 2018

dé·jà vu / ˌdāzhä ˈvoō/ • n. a feeling of having already experienced the present situation. ∎  tedious familiarity.

déjà vu

views updated May 18 2018

déjà vu (day-zha-vew) n. a vivid psychic experience in which immediately contemporary events seem to be a repetition of previous happenings. It is a symptom of some forms of epilepsy. See also jamais vu.

More From encyclopedia.com