Categorization and Concept Learning
CATEGORIZATION AND CONCEPT LEARNING
Education in every form entails the acquisition and modification of conventional categories and labels, as well as processes for inferring category membership. Consider these statements: "Fractions are numbers between two integers"; "Plants get energy through photosynthesis"; and "A noun is a person, place, or thing." The first claims a formal relation between well-defined number concepts. The second explains a biological concept by analogy. The third specifies (erroneously) a linguistic category. Teachers frequently make statements like these to elementary and secondary students. How, in fact, are the named concepts learned? How do concepts change with age, experience, and particularly education?
Form and Format of Conceptual Knowledge
A common misconception is that concepts are well defined, like dictionary entries. Though hundreds of concepts, particularly scientific ones, are well defined within a community of experts, most are "fuzzy" and metaphorical. Even patently well-defined categories like "odd number" are treated as if some examples are better than others.
Adults' concepts fall into a wide variety of abstract representations, such as taxonomic hierarchies, kinship systems, and legal definitions. Some representations mirror the structure of the physical environment, whereas others are rather arbitrary products of their cultural and linguistic environments. Most are a synthesis (e.g., biological categories reflect real patterns among organisms, yet are shaped by culturally specified theories). Children's concepts also reveal abstract representations, but these are generally less elaborate and less well-defined than those of adults.
Conceptual knowledge, as symbolic as it seems, is encoded as patterns of electrochemical activation within powerful neural networks in the neocortex. These concept patterns are derived from repeated experience. Experience trains massively interconnected systems of neural units (analogous to groups of neurons) by changing connection strengths. Over time, associated input patterns (e.g., sights and sounds of a cat) will activate a characteristic response pattern. This response is a concept (cat). Critical features of these concept patterns are graded activation (i.e., some inputs activate the response more strongly than others) and learning algorithms that specify how connection strengths change with experience. This pre-symbolic view of conceptual representation, though unintuitive, is dominant among cognitive scientists, and work in the mid-1980s through 1990s has answered some early criticisms. Still, questions remain about how conceptual thinking emerges from in neural networks. For example, it is not clear how current theoretical models can capture intricacies of conceptual knowledge (e.g., nonliteral usage of concepts, as in the ironic use of award in the "Golden Turkey Awards" for the year's worst movies).
A comprehensive proposal by Lawrence W. Barsalou in 1999 holds that concepts are inherently perceptual and experiential. So-called abstract conceptual knowledge is in fact the productive activation of remembered aspects of perceptual and internal experiences. As experiences are retained in memory, the associations among them permit new mental simulations that support a variety of functions. These functions include conceptual redescription, inference, imagination, and productive combination of concepts. These functions are traditionally ascribed to symbolic, completely abstract concepts, but Barsalou argues that they can more simply be attributed to a powerful system for manipulating stored perceptual knowledge. The power of the system rests on selective simulation: when the concept cat is activated, one does not recall every cat experience, but the experiences evoked by current contextual demands and recently activated information. Thus, the word skunk might normally activate the property smelly, but after watching a documentary showing footage of skunks foraging at night, the property nocturnal might be activated as well.
Educators should judiciously use definitions to teach new concepts. Students will not typically use definitions to judge category membership. Exposing students to multiple examples that highlight the distinctive properties of the category of interest, or comparing contrastive categories, is more effective. A common misunderstanding of concept learning can be seen in the traditional western approach to mathematics education that emphasizes abstract or "content-free" knowledge. There is no evidence of such decontextualized knowledge, and it should be assumed that mathematical concepts are derived from organized experience with concepts of quantity (e.g., cardinality, equivalence) and operations on quantities (e.g., concatenation; transformation), in a variety of familiar materials. Although it is trivially apparent that diverse, rich experiences eventually increase knowledge of mathematical and scientific concepts, the general, powerful principles for optimizing the presentation of multiple examples in classrooms, so that students' conceptual knowledge is effectively and efficiently enriched, have not yet been derived.
Theories Of Category Learning
How do experiences give rise to new categories? Presumably individuals are exposed to various members, and sometimes told about category membership. Parents use various strategies for teaching children new categories, category labels, and associated properties. But how does experience generate new categories?
In the 1994 model of Robert M. Nosofsky and his colleagues, people distinguish contrasting categories by gradually modifying the degree of attention allocated to various features of possible category members, until the simplest and most diagnostic decision rule (i.e., set of attention "weights" focusing on the fewest possible features) is attained. Atypical category members (e.g., ostriches, which differ from typical North American birds in many regards) are represented separately, as exceptions. Current controversy focuses on the information retained from multiple experiences, exact algorithms for describing changes in represented categories with experience, and the uniformity of category learning across domains. For example, there is evidence that people treat atypical examples differently across the domains of natural, object, and social categories.
How do children learn categories? Little research addresses changes in category learning processes during and after childhood. Research on children's categories often confounds their conceptual knowledge with their comprehension of category labels. A current debate concerns what categories children acquire first. Though prelinguistic infants differentiate related categories (e.g., cats vs. lions ), their knowledge might not transcend surface similarity. Knowledge of the dynamic or hidden properties of categories becomes evident after the first six months, but two-year-olds still sometimes fail to differentiate related categories (e.g., calling any water fowl duckie ) or categorize different-looking entities. In such cases parents can shape children's naming. Children, however, select and organize social input about categories and labels. For example, parents predominantly use basic-level labels when talking to children. Basic-level categories (e.g., car, bird ) capture a useful intermediate level of generality, compared to very broad (e.g., animal ) or narrow (e.g., parakeet ) categories. Compared to parents, however, preschool children use a much higher proportion of basic level labels, suggesting that children's inductive dispositions (or limitations) shape their acquisition and use of category labels.
Adults can help children focus on similarities of, and variability between, category members. Both social input and selection and tailoring of available examples can facilitate category learning. An unresolved question is how analog (i.e., rich, realistic), versus digital (i.e., reduced, electronic), examples can facilitate children's concept learning. This is a practical question, given the growing availability of computer technology for young children. A problem is that children sometimes mistake which features define a category (e.g., rejecting a barren island but accepting a tropical peninsula as examples of island). Predicting children's misconceptions about categories is therefore crucial for teachers. Verbal instruction about critical features can be ineffective or misleading, so teachers must orchestrate creative experiences and instruction to advance children's grasp of conventional categories.
Conceptual Change in Children
Children, adolescents, and adults enter classrooms with naive preconceptions about the world. Shifting these preconceptions can be onerous. In 1985 Susan Carey documented children's changing biological concepts and related inferences about biological properties. For example, a child who conceptualizes people as prototypes of animals will extend traits of humans (e.g., respiring, sleeping) to similar creatures (e.g., gorillas), but not dissimilar ones (e.g., worms). In contrast, adults often generalize biological properties in a less anthropocentric manner.
The process of conceptual change is mysterious, and frustrates educators' desires to facilitate it. Researchers have suggested many procedures to promote conceptual change, but naive concepts can be extraordinarily intractable. For example, even after formal physics instruction, older students make pervasive errors about concepts like force. One approach to teaching stipulates having students articulate their concepts, setting up a demonstration that would yield different results under the naive and the conventional belief system, having students predict an outcome, and then conducting the demonstration. Apparently articulating and confronting the discrepancy between own and others' beliefs, and seeing relevant evidence, can provide a powerful springboard for conceptual change. Note, however, that this approach is effective with older students revising well-defined concepts in a mature science. Its effectiveness for young children, whose capacity to recognize disconfirming evidence is limited, has not been established.
The Functions of Categorization in Children'S Thinking
The content of children's categories changes with age, but what of the uses of categorization and concepts? Concepts constrain perception, language, social interaction, and problem solving–every aspect of cognition. Categories permit economic thinking, inferring properties of novel instances, organizing memories, making analogies, and solving problems with flexibility. It is not known how education, informal learning, and maturation separately alter these functions. In short, there is a lack of a developmental theory of the ecology of categorization that takes into account development and schooling. Outlining this theory is a major task for developmental and educational psychology in the twenty-first century.
See also: Language Acquisition; Learning, subentry on Conceptual Change.
bibliography
Barsalou, Lawrence W. 1999. "Perceptual Symbol Systems." Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22:577–660.
Bransford, John D.; Franks, Jeffery J.; Vye, Nancy. J.; and Sherwood, Robert D. 1989. "New Approaches to Instruction: Because Wisdom Can't Be Told." In Similarity and Analogical Reasoning, ed. Stella Vosnaidou and Andrew Ortony. Cambridge, Eng.: Cambridge University Press.
Carey, Susan. 1985. Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chi, Micheline T. H.; Hutchinson, Jean E.; and Robin, Anne F. 1989. "How Inferences about Novel Domain-Related Concepts Can Be Constrained by Structured Knowledge." Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 35:27–62.
DeÁk, Gedeon O. 2000. "Chasing the Fox of Word Learning: Why 'Constraints' Fail to Capture It." Developmental Review 20:29–80.
Gelman, Susan A., et al. 1998. Beyond Labeling: The Role of Maternal Input in the Acquisition of Richly Structured Categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 63 (1). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Hunt, Earl, and Minstrell, Jim. 1994. "A Cognitive Approach to the Teaching of Physics." In Classroom Lessons: Integrating Cognitive Theory and Classroom Practices, ed. Kate McGilly. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Keil, Frank C., and Batterman, Nancy. 1984. "A Characteristic-to-Defining Shift in the Development of Word Meaning." Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23:221–236.
Lakoff, George. 1987. Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Livingston, Kenneth R., and Andrews, Janet K. 1995. "On the Interaction of Prior Knowledge and Stimulus Structure in Category Learning." Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 48:208–236.
Mandler, Jean M. 2000. "Perceptual and Conceptual Processes in Infancy." Journal of Cognition and Development 1:3–36.
Medin, Douglas L.; Lynch, Elizabeth B.; and Solomon, Karen O. 2000. "Are There Kinds of Concepts?" Annual Review of Psychology 51:121–147.
Mervis, Carolyn B. 1987. "Child-Basic Object Categories and Early Lexical Development." In Concepts and Conceptual Development, ed. Ulric Neisser. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Murphy, George L., and Medin, Douglas L. 1985. "The Role of Theories in Conceptual Coherence." Psychological Review 92:289–316.
Nosofsky, Robert M.; Palmeri, Thomas J.; and McKinley, Stephen C. 1994. "Rule-Plus-Exception Model of Classification Learning." Psychological Review 101:53–79.
Rosch, Eleanor. 1978. "Principles of Categorization." In Cognition and Categorization, ed. Eleanor Rosch and Barbara Lloyd. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Rumelhart, David E., and McClelland, James L. 1989. Parallel Distributed Processing: Explorations in the Microstructure of Cognition, Vol. 1. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Gedeon O. DeÁk
More From encyclopedia.com
About this article
Categorization and Concept Learning
You Might Also Like
NEARBY TERMS
Categorization and Concept Learning