Qualitative Methods
QUALITATIVE METHODS
The term qualitative methods refers to a variety of research techniques and procedures associated with the goal of trying to understand the complexities of the social world in which we live and how we go about thinking, acting, and making meaning in our lives. These research practices, which emphasize getting close to participants and trying to understand how they (and we) view the world, include, among others, participant observation, interviews, life histories, and focus groups; autoethnographic, phenomenological, narrative, and most ethnomethodological and feminist approaches; particular forms of documentary, content, discourse, and conversational analysis research; and some action research.
Qualitative researchers may be placed along a broad continuum ranging from an orientation akin to positivist science to one more akin to art and literature. In between is a vast middle ground where elements of both orientations are present. Moving along the qualitative continuum from science to art and literature, one finds practitioners who see social life as something out there to be discovered independently of the researcher, those who view social life as something constructed through interaction and engagement with the world, and those who focus more closely on the person describing social life and the modes and practices of description (see Crotty 1998; Denzin 1997). Across the continuum, the focus changes from studying others who are assumed to be uniquely separate from the researcher, to examining interactions between the researcher and others, to including the positionality, politics, and story of the researcher who interacts with others.
Currently qualitative work enjoys a burgeoning interest across social science disciplines including anthropology, sociology, communication, education, social work, and nursing. The result is a growing sense of a qualitative community unconstrained by disciplinary boundaries. As this community grows and forms its identity, the spectrum of possibilities broadens, creating new alternatives for qualitative research and, in the process, raising vexing and controversial questions (see Denzin and Lincoln 1995; Snow and Morrill 1995). Given the interpretive turn (Rabinow and Sullivan 1987) in social science, more and more researchers are applying art-based criteria to their work; at the same time, new computer programs, such as NVivo, allow for more systematic and rigorous coding of qualitative data. We view these differences and ensuing conversations as strengthening qualitative research and representing a coming of age for qualitative work (Bochner and Ellis in press).
We organize our discussion of qualitative methods according to three ideal types representing points on the qualitative continuum from the science/causal pole through the middle ground to the artful/interpretive pole. These categories are intended as a useful means of dividing the territory, but they are also arbitrary and should not be taken as a literal map of the field. Rather, we encourage readers to envision a wide expanse of methodological approaches and to view the boundaries we have constructed as permeable, flexible, and fleeting, with many qualitative researchers likely to position themselves in more than one category. As the authors, both of us have engaged in middle ground and artful/interpretive qualitative work; our current allegiance lies primarily in the artful/interpretive region of the continuum.
QUALITATIVE RESEARCH AS SCIENCE
At one end of the qualitative spectrum, researchers approach qualitative research as an extension of quantitative inquiry. Their goal is to produce propositional knowledge about human behavior generalizable to specific populations. They see truth as something "out there" to be discovered and explained by the researcher. Positioning themselves as neutral, disinterested parties in the research process, they want to be able to control and predict subsequent behavior of samples within the population being investigated. These researchers follow the building-block, foundational model of scientific knowledge, viewing inquiry as a linear progression, with each new discovery adding on to available explanations. Qualitative researchers in this tradition express many of the same concerns as their quantitative counterparts, including an interest in random sampling, reliability, validity, and ethical issues. The language they use to present and discuss results applies many of the familiar lines of the hypothetico-deductive model.
Random sampling of the studied population (regardless of specific research tools to be used) provides assurance that the qualitative researcher has obtained a representative and unbiased sample of research subjects (Lindlof 1995, p. 121). Since researchers want to claim that their findings can be generalized to the selected population, it is critical that the demographic characteristics of the sample match those of the population defined in the study. For example, Lowry and Towles (1989), in their study of the portrayal of sex and its consequences in afternoon soap operas, randomly sampled episodes of soap operas from each TV network in order to be able to draw conclusions about soap opera content in general.
Using an approach similar to that of quantitative research, qualitative researchers in this tradition examine variables that relate to specific behaviors, traits, or characteristics that are narrowly defined in as specific a manner as possible. They manipulate the independent variables and measure the outcome of the experiment in terms of dependent variables, those defined behaviors, traits, or characteristics thought to exist in relationship to the independent variables. Before they conduct experiments, researchers form a hypothesis about the relationship between the variables. Data interpretation then centers on determining whether the hypotheses are negated by the results; researchers do not generally examine data for themes or issues unrelated to the predetermined focuses of the study. Based on a review of relevant literature, Chavez (1985), for example, hypothesized that writers treat men and women differently in comic strips. She then collected a sample of comic strips, coded the gender, settings, and activities of the characters, and concluded that her hypothesis was supported.
This process of hypothesis formation and testing often is less formal, however, even among those striving to maintain a scientific approach to their research, and it can take many different forms. In the study of the realism of aggression on television by Potter et al. (1995), for example, the authors laid out a set of premises about what would constitute a realistic (similar to the real world levels of violence in numbers and context) portrayal of aggression. They then collected a sample of television programming, coded the various acts of aggression, and compared the numbers and types of aggression in their sample to the premises they had developed.
Even those researchers who do not convert their data into numerical form often go to great lengths to assure that their findings are valid and reliable. For researchers at this end of the qualitative continuum, validity means that the concepts they examine are those intended to be examined and not confounded or overlapping with other concepts not intended to be included. A study is reliable if researchers find, or could expect to find, the same, or very similar, results when they conduct the study again For example, Waitzkin (1990) provides criteria to establish reliability and validity of qualitative data: (1) discourse should be selected through a sampling procedure, preferably a randomized technique; (2) recordings of sampled discourse should be available for review by other observers; (3) standardized rules of transcription should be used; (4) the reliability of transcription should be assessed by multiple observers; (5) procedures of interpretation should be decided in advance, should be validated in relation to theory, and should address both content and structure of texts; (6) the reliability of applying interpretive procedures should be assessed by multiple observers; (7) a summary and excerpts from transcripts should accompany the interpretation, but full transcripts should also be available for review; and (8) texts and interpretations should convey the variability of content and structure across sampled texts (pp. 480–482).
Waitzkin's criteria (1990) emphasize three specific concerns associated with reliability and validity from a scientific perspective. First, the collective decisions and interpretations of multiple researchers would be closer to an "objective" reality than a presumably more biased perspective of a single individual. Second, it is important to consider the whole body of available data as the basis for interpretation to avoid making general statements that reflect only a subset of the data; the emphasis is on what is common throughout the data, not on that which is unusual, distinctive, or unique. Third, written transcripts must be publicly available for verification.
Qualitative researchers working in this tradition use a system of coding to categorize videotaped or observed behaviors, written responses to survey questions, verbal responses to an interviewer, or other data (Wimmer and Dominick 1997). Once labeled, the observed behaviors can be counted, sorted, and analyzed statistically. Coding schema can be standardized typologies that are used by other researchers, or they can be developed in light of a specific research question or body of data. Chavez (1985), mentioned above, developed a typology of settings and activities (e.g., child care or working in an office) for the cartoon characters based on what she found in her comic strip data set. To aid in the analysis of transcript data, specialized computer software programs, such as NUD*IST, Ethnograph, or NVivo (a new program that integrates text, image, sound, and video data sets), are available. A critical component to coding is establishing intercoder agreement; that is, a measure to ensure that the coding schema can be consistently applied to the same set of data by different coders and the same or very similar results obtained (Wimmer and Dominick 1997).
Ethical issues at this end of the continuum, similar to those in quantitative research, focus on methodological procedures, in particular honesty and thoroughness in data collection and analysis. Authors often elaborately spell out their research procedures in their publications, making the procedures and data available for scrutiny in order to justify claims or conclusions they draw. In science-oriented qualitative research, authors stay behind the scenes, portraying themselves as trustworthy and credible through their disembodied discussion of methods without showing in their texts their own involvement or self-interest.
Those engaging in scientific approaches to qualitative research usually adhere closely to the writing style used by quantitative researchers. A passive voice shadows the presence of the author and obscures the "I" of the researcher (Gergen 1994). Statements such as "It was found that . . ." and "The data revealed that . . ." reinforce the notion of neutral authors who have discovered preexisting ideas, and who, without contaminating the material with their own perspectives, then pass it along for readers to receive as knowledge. Of course, researchers who see their work as scientific often acknowledge that the author is not a blank slate without values and beliefs, but their use of a disinterested, passive voice remains a sign of how important they view the ideal of distance and objectivity, even if it is not fully attainable.
MIDDLE-GROUND APPROACHES TO RESEARCH
Between science and art, one finds a sprawling middle ground of qualitative researchers, who seek to analyze events, find patterns, and create models from their data (Neuman 1997). Here, researchers do not adhere rigidly to the rules of empiricism; but they are not likely to experiment with narrative, poetic, or literary forms of writing either. In the middle, researchers seek some combination of scientific rigor and artistic imagination. How these two goals intersect differs for various researchers and often connects to the author's specific location relative to art and science on the qualitative continuum.
Middle-ground researchers use a variety of methodologies to gather data for analysis, including unstructured or semistructured interviewing (Fontana and Frey 1994; Mishler 1986), focus groups (Kitzinger 1994), participant observation or fieldwork (Lincoln and Guba 1985; Lofland and Lofland 1995), textual analysis (Reinharz 1992), and analysis of narrative (M. M. Gergen 1992; Riessman 1993). While there are many ways to go about selecting a sample, those in the middle ground of qualitative research often use purposeful sampling (Miles and Huberman 1984), in which they try to obtain cases that are information rich, or a "snowball" approach (Reinharz 1992), in which they ask current participants to identify other possible participants.
One of the most useful and widely applied strategies associated with the middle ground is an approach, developed by Glaser and Strauss (1967), called grounded theory. In this approach, researchers emphasize the generation of categories and theory from systematic coding and analysis of qualitative data (often transcripts) (Charmaz 1990; Glaser and Strauss 1967; Janesick 1994). This method of inductive reasoning differs from traditional social science in which researchers use previously established theory and test it deductively with data to see whether the theory can be sustained.
Methodological concerns are very important to grounded theory researchers, who often hold to the belief that if you apply a valid and systematic methodological approach, you'll get closer to an accurate representation of what's actually going on. In analyzing the data, some adhere rigidly to formal steps in grounded theory research—data notes, sort and classify, open coding, axial coding, selective coding, with memo writing occurring throughout the process (Charmaz 1990; Neuman 1997; Strauss and Corbin 1994). The more the researcher adheres to systematic analysis, the greater the likelihood of using computer programs to assist in coding. Other theorists, in the middle of the continuum, who think of themselves as grounded theorists, "eyeball" the data in less systematic ways, perhaps not even completely transcribing or coding data into categories. Yet they too seek patterns among the data they have collected, though they view the process as more subjective and intuitive than scientific and objective.
While some middle-ground researchers may place less emphasis on scientific precision, they usually adhere to criteria or guidelines concerning the processes of data analysis, though these rules may vary widely (see, for example, Charmaz 1997; Glaser 1978; Strauss and Corbin 1997). Tompkins (1994), for example, refers to representativeness, consistency (for public and private texts), and recalcitrance (sanction by research participants or a similar group) as standards for evaluating data used in qualitative research (see also Fitch 1994). Working closer to the interpretive pole, Lather (1986) argues that validity in openly ideological research can be established through four guidelines: triangulation of multiple data sources, methods, and theoretical perspective; assessment of construct validity through use of systematized reflexivity between the daily experiences of people and the theoretical constructs being developed; establishment of face validity through sharing analysis with respondents and refining analysis with their input; and determination of catalytic validity, that is the potential for bringing about positive change and social transformation (p. 67). These different criteria are similar insofar as they provide standards for bridging researcher and participant perspectives, so that findings reflect the meanings of the people whose lives were examined.
Ethical issues for those in the middle group focus on research practices such as covert research, deception, informed consent, anonymity, confidentiality, and revealing knowledge about the less powerful. These issues then lead to ethical questions about what should be studied, how, and by whom (see Lofland and Lofland 1995).
Most middle-ground researchers note the positionality of participants, such as race, class, and sexual orientation, in order to avoid obscuring these factors. For example, Ellingson and Buzzanell (1999) studied a group of white, middle-class, heterosexual breast cancer survivors in a small Midwestern city. They acknowledged that these demographic characteristics impacted the results of the study; a more racially mixed group, or a group composed of lesbians, for instance, most likely would have produced a different set of findings.
Just as those in the middle ground acknowledge the positionality of participants, they also sometimes acknowledge their standpoint, personal background, politics, and interests in the topic (Collins 1991). Insofar as they see knowledge as "constructed" rather than discovered, middle-ground researchers discuss their personal perspectives or political commitments as an acknowledgment that all knowledge is generated from a specific social position and reflects the perspectives of the researchers involved. Researchers in the middle ground may try to decrease the power disparity between themselves and the people they study (DeVault 1990; Ellingson and Buzzanell 1999). They generally refer to those in the study as research participants or informants rather than subjects, indicating a degree of respect for the people whose lives are being studied (Reinharz 1992).
In the middle ground, researchers study a variety of topics, including complex issues that are difficult or impossible to address with quantitative methodology, such as understanding hierarchy in groups (Whyte [1943] 1993) or awareness contexts in death (Glaser and Strauss 1964). Some seek to make visible previously invisible aspects of the lives of women and other groups underrepresented in traditional social scientific research such as ethnic and racial minorities, gays and lesbians, and people with disabilities (Spitzack and Carter 1989). Others examine groups that are hidden, unknown, or inaccessible to most people, such as mushroom gatherers (Fine 1992) or white supremists (Mitchell 1998). As the topics get more complex and oriented toward meanings, subjectivity, and emotionality, it becomes more difficult to invoke older, more traditional, systematic "scientific methods" and apply them.
Writers in this tradition alter some of the conventions of scientific writing. They may include standpoint statements within the introductory section of articles, indicating their personal interest in the topic. They may use a conventional format but preface the article with vignettes or include excerpts from participant narratives in the discussion of findings or in an appendix to add texture and authenticity to the work. To acknowledge their presence in the work, authors may write in the first person. Nevertheless, researchers in this tradition usually privilege theory generation, typicality, and generalization to a wider world over evocative storytelling, concrete experience, and multiple perspectives that include participants' voices and interpretations. They tend to write realist tales in an authorial, omnipotent voice. Snippets of fieldwork data then represent participants' stories, primarily valued for illustrating general concepts, patterns, and themes (see Van Maanen 1988).
RESEARCH AS ARTISTIC ENDEAVOR
During the last two decades, many qualitative researchers have moved toward an emphasis on the artistic aspects of qualitative work (Wolcott 1995). Working from an orientation that blends the practices and emphases of social science with the aesthetic sensibility and expressive forms of art, these researchers seek to tell stories that show experience as lived in all its bodily, cognitive, emotional, and spiritual aspects. The goal is to practice an artful, poetic, and empathic social science in which readers can keep in their minds and feel in their bodies the complexities of concrete moments of lived experience. These writers want readers to be able to put themselves in the place of others, within a culture of experience that enlarges their social awareness and empathy. Their goals include: evoking emotional experience in readers (Ellis 1997); giving voice to stories and groups of people traditionally left out of social scientific inquiry (DeVault 1990); producing writing of high literary/artistic quality (Richardson in press); and improving readers', participants', and authors' lives (see Denzin 1997; Fine 1994).
According to Bochner, Ellis, and their colleagues (Bochner 1994; Bochner et al. 1998; Ellis 1997), the interpretive, narrative, autoethnographic project has the following distinguishing features: the author usually writes in the first person, making herself or himself the object of research (Jackson 1989; Tedlock 1991); the narrative text focuses on generalization within a single case extended over time (Geertz 1973); the text is presented as a story replete with a narrator, characterization, and plot line, akin to forms of writing associated with the novel or biography; the story often discloses hidden details of private life and highlights emotional experience; the ebb and flow of relationship experience is depicted in an episodic form that dramatizes the motion of connected lives across the curve of time; a reflexive connection exists between the lives of participants and researchers that must be explored; and the relationships between writers and readers of the texts is one of involvement and participation.
Rather than believing in the presence of an external, unconstructed truth, researchers on this end of the continuum embrace narrative truth (Spence 1982), which means that the experience as described is believable, lifelike, and possible. Through narrative we learn to understand the meanings and significance of the past as incomplete, tentative, and revisable according to contingencies of present life circumstances (Crites 1971). In this research, authors are concerned about issues of validity, reliability, and generalizability, but these issues may be redefined to meet the goals of the research.
Ellis and Bochner (in press), for example, define validity to mean that the work resonates with readers and evokes in them a feeling that the experience has verisimilitude. A story is valid if it stimulates readers to enter the experience described or to feel and think about their own, whether in memory, anticipated, or lived. Validity might be judged by whether it offers readers assistance in communicating with others different from themselves, or a way to improve the lives of participants and readers or even the author's own. Since writers always create their personal narrative from a situated location, trying to make their present, imagined future, and remembered past cohere, orthodox reliability does not exist in narrative research. But reliability checks are still important. Researchers often take their work back to participants and give them a chance to comment, add materials, change their minds, and offer their interpretations. Since we all participate in a limited number of cultures and institutions, lives are typical and generalizable, as well as particular. A story's generalizability is constantly being tested by readers as they determine whether the story speaks to them about their own experiences or about the experiences of others they know. Likewise, does it tell them about people or lives about which they are unfamiliar? Does a work bring "felt" news from one world to another and provide opportunities for the reader to have vicarious experience of the things told (Stake 1994)?
Interpretive research reflects the messiness of lived experience and emotions. Unlike researchers at the scientific end and in the middle ground, artful or interpretive researchers do not look for common denominators and generalities, but instead examine experience that is unique, particular, moving, and possible. They embrace their own subjectivity not to acknowledge bias but to celebrate positionality and their particular construction of the world. While some of these writers employ traditional analysis in their work, examining stories for concepts, patterns, and themes, others argue for the importance of thinking with a story, not just about a story. Thinking with a story means to allow yourself to resonate with the story, reflect on it, become a part of it (see Frank 1995b). Others argue that theory is embedded in the story (Ellis 1995b), that all good stories make a theoretical point.
Arguments about methods are not nearly as prevalent here as in the other two groups. Methods articles are much more likely to emphasize flexibility and emergence than to offer strict rules for how to proceed (Ellis et al. 1997). One of the most discussed methodological issues is whether researchers should attempt to follow the rules of traditional ethnographic methods or whether narrative research should be approached more like writing fiction. One's position on that question most likely intersects with one's stance regarding the role of criteria in evaluating narrative writing. In the former, traditional ethnographic criteria might be more commonplace; in the latter, narratives might be judged by their usefulness, the compassion they encourage, and the dialogue they promote (Ellis and Bochner in press). Seeking to position interpretive texts as an intersection of social science and art form, Richardson (in press) discusses five criteria she uses to evaluate interpretive ethnography: (1) Substantive contribution: Does the work contribute to an understanding of the text? (2) Aesthetic merit: Is the text artistically shaped, complex, and satisfying? (3) Reflexivity: Does the writer reflect on the production of the text so that the reader can make judgments about the point of view employed in the text? (4) Impactfulness: Does this work affect me and move me to respond or act? (5) Expression of Reality: Does this work seem a credible account of the real?
Rather than method and criteria, most articles about interpretive/artful ethnography grapple with issues of writing. For narrative researchers, writing (the form) is inseparable from the process of data interpretation (the content). As Richardson (in press) phrases it, writing in interpretive qualitative research is not a "mopping-up" activity at the end of a research project but an integral part of data analysis; authors write to find out what they have to say about their data and their experiences. The voice of the author is as central to the text as the voices of those "being studied." Writers experiment with approaches and writing conventions to dislodge assumptions about science and incorporate the artistic into representations of data.
Interpretive research embraces a range of processes and approaches, including biographical method (Denzin 1989), observation of participation (Tedlock 1991), ethnography (Van Maanen 1988), autoethnography (Ellingson 1998; Reed-Danahay 1997), interactive interviewing (Ellis et al. 1997), systematic sociological introspection (Ellis 1991), co-constructed methods (Bochner and Ellis 1992), personal experience methods (Clandinin and Connelly 1994), narrative analysis (Bochner 1994), and feminist methods (Reinharz 1992).
Examples of creative representation include layered accounts (Ronai 1995), ethnographic fiction (Angrosino 1998), personal essays (Krieger 1991), impressionist tales (Van Maanen 1988), coconstructed narratives (Bochner and Ellis 1992), poetic representation of data (Austin 1996; Glesne 1997; Richardson 1992, 1994), writing stories (Richardson 1997), ethnographic performance or ethnodrama (Jones 1998; Mienczakowski 1996), and polyvocal texts (Lather and Smithies 1997).
A particularly controversial narrative writing practice and form is autoethnography, which is an autobiographical genre of writing and research that displays multiple layers of consciousness as it connects the personal to the cultural. Several autoethnographic genres currently exist side by side (Ellis and Bochner in press): (1) Reflexive ethnographies focus on a culture or subculture, but authors use their own experiences in the culture reflexively to bend back on self and look more deeply at self-other interactions. Reflexive ethnographers ideally use all their senses, their bodies, moment, feeling, and their whole being to learn about the other (Jackson 1989). (2) In native ethnographies, researchers who are natives of cultures that have been marginalized or exoticized by others write about and interpret their own cultures for others. (3) In personal narratives, social scientists take on the dual identities of academic and personal selves to tell autobiographical stories about some aspect of their daily life.
In all these forms of qualitative writing, narrative researchers use fiction-writing techniques such as dramatic recall, dialogue, flashback, strong imagery, scene setting, character development, interior monologue, suspense, and a dramatic plot line that is developed through the specific actions of specific characters with specific bodies doing specific things. They ask readers to relive the events with the writer and then to reflect on their emotional responses and life experiences, and on the moral questions and concerns that arise. Yet, the work differs from fiction in that the writing and publishing conventions used arise out of social science traditions, and in that the work often, though not always, has more of an overt analytic purpose and more of an analytic frame than fiction has.
Ethical concerns include matters of how we go about doing our research and what we owe those who become characters in and readers of our stories (Ellis 1995a). How do we deal with issues of confidentiality when including people, such as family members, who can be easily identified? What do we do if characters in our stories disagree with our interpretations or want us to omit material about them? How do we include multiple voices and perspectives within our stories? What is the role of the traditional authorial voice of the author? How do we stay true to our participants yet not deceive our readers (Josselson 1996; see also Chase 1996)? Is there value in working from an ethic of care, empathy, and responsibility, rather than informed consent, and is that ever possible in the world of research (Collins 1991; Denzin 1997)? How do we make our projects therapeutic for ourselves as well as our participants and readers? What do we want the world to be? How can we contribute to making it that way and, in the process, become better human beings?
CONCLUSION
Qualitative methods is a rich and varied set of approaches to research. Journals such as Qualitative Inquiry, Qualitative Sociology, Symbolic Interaction, and the Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, as well as a number of research annuals such as Studies in Symbolic Interaction and Cultural Studies: A Research Annual, along with subfield-specific journals, such as Qualitative Health Research, showcase examples of qualitative research in sociology and provide forums for discussion of methodological issues. The joy (and sometimes the frustration!) of qualitative methods is the promotion and valuing of a wide spectrum of methods, none of which should be viewed as the only way to conduct research in sociology. It might be more comforting if there was one set of rules to follow, but that comfort would come with the tragic price of close-mindedness, silencing of voices, and narrowing of vision. We agree with Rorty (1982), who says we ought to learn to live with differences without feeling we have to resolve them.
references
Angrosino, M. V. 1998 Opportunity House: EthnographicStories of Mental Retardation. Walnut Creek, Calif.: AltaMira.
Austin, D. A. 1996 "Kaleidoscope: The Same and Different." In C. Ellis and A. P. Bochner, eds., ComposingEthnography: Alternative Forms of Qualitative Writing. Walnut Creek, Calif.: AltaMira.
Bochner, A. P. 1994 "Perspectives on Inquiry II: Theories and Stories." In M. Knapp and G. R. Miller, eds., The Handbook of Interpersonal Communication, 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
——, and C. Ellis 1992 "Personal Narrative as a Social Approach to Interpersonal Communication." Communication Theory 2:165–172.
—— in press "Which Way to Turn?" Journal of Contemporary Ethnography.
——, and L. Tillmann-Healy 1998 "Mucking Around Looking for Truth." In B. Montgomery and L. Baxter, eds., Dialectical Approaches to Studying Personal Relationships. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Chase, S. 1996. "Personal Vulnerability and Interpretive Authority in Narrative Research." In R. Josselson, ed., Ethics and Process in The Narrative Study of Lives, vol. 4. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Charmaz, K. 1990 "'Discovering' Chronic Illness: Using Grounded Theory." Sociology of Health and Illness 30:1161–1172.
Chavez, D. 1985 "Perpetuation of Gender Inequality: A Content Analysis of Comic Strips." Sex Roles 13: 93–102.
Clandinin, D. J., and F. M. Connelly 1994 "Personal Experience Methods." In N. K. Denzin and Y. S. Lincoln, eds., Handbook of Qualitative Research. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Collins, P. H. 1991 Black Feminist Thought. New York: Routledge.
Crites, S. 1971 "The Narrative Quality of Experience." Journal of the American Academy of Religion 39:291–311.
Crotty, M. 1998 The Foundations of Social Research. London: Sage.
Denzin, N. 1989 Interpretive Biography. Newbury Park, Calif.: Sage.
—— 1997 Interpretive Ethnography: Ethnographic Practices for the 21st Century. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
——, and Y. Lincoln 1995 "Transforming Qualitative Research Methods: Is It a Revolution?" Journal ofContemporary Ethnography 24:349–358.
DeVault, M. L. 1990 "Talking and Listening from Women's Standpoint: Feminist Strategies for Interviewing and Analysis." Social Problems 37:96–116.
Ellingson, L. L. 1998 "'Then You Know How I Feel': Empathy, Identification, and Reflexivity in Fieldwork." Qualitative Inquiry 4:492–514.
——, and P. M. Buzzanell 1999 "Listening to Women's Narratives of Breast Cancer Treatment: A Feminist Approach to Patient Satisfaction with Physician-Patient Communication." Health Communication 11:153–183.
Ellis, C. 1991 "Sociological Introspection and Emotional Experience." Symbolic Interaction 14:23–50.
—— 1995a "Emotional and Ethical Quagmires in Returning to the Field." Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 24:711–713.
—— 1995b Final Negotiations: A Story of Love, Loss, andChronic Illness. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.
—— 1997 "Evocative Autoethnography: Writing Emotionally about Our Lives." In W. Tierney and Y. Lincoln, eds., Representation and the Text: Reframingthe Narrative Voice. Albany, N.Y.: SUNY Press.
——, and A. Bochner in press "Autoethnography, Personal Narrative, Reflexivity: Researcher as Subject." In N. K. Denzin and Y. S. Lincoln, eds., Handbook of Qualitative Research, 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Ellis, C., K. E. Kiesinger, and L. M. Tillmann-Healy 1997 "Interactive Interviewing: Talking about Emotional Experience." In R. Hertz, ed., Reflexivity and Voice. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Fine, G. 1992 "Wild Life: Authenticity and the Human Experience of 'Natural' Places." In C. Ellis and M. Flaherty, eds., Investigating Subjectivity: Research onLived Experience. Newbury Park, Calif.: Sage.
Fine, M. 1994 "Working the Hyphens: Reinventing Self and Other in Qualitative Research." In N. K. Denzin and Y. S. Lincoln, eds., Handbook of Qualitative Research. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Fitch, K. L. 1994 "Criteria for Evidence in Qualitative Research." Western Journal of Communication 58:32–38.
Fontana, A., and J. Frey 1994 "Interviewing: The Art of Science." In N. K. Denzin and Y. S. Lincoln, eds.,Handbook of Qualitative Research. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Frank, A. 1995 The Wounded Storyteller: Body, Illness, andEthics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Geertz, C. 1973 The Interpretation of Cultures: SelectedEssays. New York: Basic Books.
Gergen, K. J. 1994 Realities and Relationships: Soundingsin Social Construction. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Gergen, M. M. 1992 "Life Stories: Pieces of a Dream." In G. C. Rosenwald and R. L. Ochberg, eds., StoriedLives: The Cultural Politics of Self-Understanding. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University Press.
Glaser, B. 1978 Theoretical Sensitivity. Mill Valley, Calif.: Sociological Press.
——, and B. Strauss 1964 Awareness of Dying. Chicago: Aldine.
—— 1967 The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategiesfor Qualitative Research. Chicago: Aldine.
Glesne, C. 1997 "That Rare Feeling: Re-Presenting Research through Poetic Transcription." QualitativeInquiry 3:202–221.
Jackson, M. 1989 Paths toward a Clearing: Radical Empiricism and Ethnographic Inquiry. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Janesick, V. J. 1994 "The Dance of Qualitative Research Design: Metaphor, Methodology, and Meaning." In N. K. Denzin and Y. S. Lincoln, eds., Handbook ofQualitative Research. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Jones, S. H. 1998 Kaleidoscope Notes: Writing Women'sMusic and Organizational Culture. Walnut Creek, Calif.: AltaMira.
Josselson, R. 1996. "On Writing Other People's Lives: Self-Analytic Reflections of a Narrative Researcher." In R. Josselson, ed., Ethics and Process in the NarrativeStudy of Lives, vol. 4. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Kitzinger, J. 1994 "The Methodology of Focus Groups: The Importance of Interaction between Research Participants." Sociology of Health and Illness 16:103–121.
Krieger, S. 1991 Social Science and the Self: Personal Essayson an Art Form. New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University Press.
Lather, P. 1986 "Research as Praxis." Harvard Educational Review 56:257–277.
——, and C. Smithies 1997 Troubling the Angels: Women Living with HIV/AIDS. Boulder, Colo.: Westview.
Lincoln, Y. S., and E. Guba 1985 Naturalistic Inquiry. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage.
Lindlof, T. R. 1995 Qualitative Communication ResearchMethods. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Lofland, J., and L. H. Lofland 1995 Analyzing SocialSettings: A Guide to Qualitative Observation and Analysis, 3rd ed. Belmont, Calif.: Wadsworth.
Lowry, D. T., and D. E. Towles 1989 "Soap Opera Portrayals of Sex, Contraception, and Sexually Transmitted Diseases." Journal of Communication 39:76–83.
Mienczakowski, J. 1996 "An Ethnographic Act: The Construction of Consensual Theatre." In C. Ellis and A. P. Bochner, eds., Composing Ethnography: Alternative Forms of Qualitative Writing. Walnut Creek, Calif.: AltaMira.
Miles, M., and A. M. Huberman 1984 Qualitative DataAnalysis: A Sourcebook of New Methods. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage.
Mishler, E. G. 1986 Research Interviewing: Context andNarrative. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Mitchell, R. 1998 "Thirteen Stars and Chocolate Cake: Reasoning with Apocalypse," Paper presented to the Society for the Study of Symbolic Interaction, San Francisco.
Neuman, W. L. 1997 Social Research Methods: Qualitativeand Quantitative Approaches, 3rd ed. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Potter, W. J., M. W. Vaughan, R. Warren, K. Howley, A. Land, and J. C. Hagemeyer 1995 "How Real Is the Portrayal of Aggression in Television Entertainment Programming?" Journal of Broadcasting and ElectronicMedia 39:494–516.
Rabinow, P., and W. Sullivan 1987 Interpretive SocialScience: A Second Look. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Reed-Danahay, D. 1997 Auto-Ethnography: Rewriting theSelf and the Social. Oxford, England: Berg.
Reinharz, S. 1992 Feminist Methods in Social Research. New York: Oxford University Press.
Richardson, L. 1992 "The Consequences of Poetic Representation." In C. Ellis and M. G. Flaherty, eds., Investigating Subjectivity: Research on Lived Experience. Newbury Park, Calif.: Sage.
—— 1994 "Nine Poems." Journal of ContemporaryEthnography 23:3–13.
—— 1997 Fields of Play: Constructing an Academic Life. New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University Press.
—— in press "Writing: A Method of Inquiry." In N. Denzin and Y. Lincoln, eds., Handbook of QualitativeResearch, 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Riessman, C. K. 1993 Narrative Analysis. QualitativeResearch Methods, vol. 30. Newbury Park, Calif.: Sage Publications.
Ronai, C. R. 1995 "Multiple Reflections of Child Sex Abuse: An Argument for a Layered Account." Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 23:395–426.
Rorty, R. 1982 Consequences of Pragmatism (Essays 1972–1980). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
Snow, D., and C. Morrill 1995 "A Revolutionary Handbook or a Handbook for Revolution?" Journal ofContemporary Ethnography 24:341–348.
Spence, D. 1982 Narrative Truth and Historical Truth. New York: Norton.
Spitzack, C., and K. Carter 1989 Doing Research onWomen's Communication: Perspectives on Theory andMethod. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex.
Stake, R. 1994 "Case Studies." In N. K. Denzin and Y. S. Lincoln, eds., The Handbook of Qualitative Research. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Strauss, A., and J. Corbin 1994 "Grounded Theory Methodology: An Overview." In N. K. Denzin and Y. S. Lincoln, eds., The Handbook of Qualitative Research. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage.
Tedlock, B. 1991 "From Participant Observation to the Observation of Participation: The Emergence of Narrative Ethnography." Journal of Anthropological Research 41:69–94.
Tompkins, P. K. 1994 "Principles of Rigor for Assessing Evidence in 'Qualitative' Communication Research." Western Journal of Communication 58:44–50.
Van Maanen, J. 1988 Tales of the Field: On WritingEthnography. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Waitzkin, H. 1990 "On Studying the Discourse of Medical Encounters: A Critique of Quantitative and Qualitative Methods and a Proposal for Reasonable Compromise. Medical Care 28:473–487.
Whyte, W. F. (1943) 1993 Street Corner Society: The SocialStructure of an Italian Slum, 5th ed., Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Wilkinson, S. 1998 "Focus Groups in Feminist Research: Power, Interaction and the Co-Construction of Meaning." Women's Studies International Forum 21:111–125.
Wimmer, R. D., and J. Dominick 1997 Mass MediaResearch: An Introduction, 5th ed. Belmont, Calif.: Wadsworth.
Wolcott, H. 1995 The Art of Fieldwork. Walnut Creek, Calif.: AltaMira.
Carolyn Ellis Laura Ellingson
Methods, Qualitative
Methods, Qualitative
RESEARCH VALIDITY, RELIABILITY, AND ETHICS
Qualitative methodology is used by social scientists in the study of human behavior. This methodology may be used in addition to or in place of quantitative methods. The use of qualitative methods by social scientists allows the researcher to obtain a rich set of data that is not easily obtainable with the use of quantitative methods. Qualitative methods encompass a variety of methodologies, including observation, interviewing, document analysis, and archival document analysis.
OBSERVATION AND INTERVIEWING
Two key methods used in the social sciences are observation and interviewing. Observation involves the examination of research subjects in the natural social environment, with particular attention paid to the subject’s behavior and actions. These observations are made firsthand by the researcher. An important element of observation is to study the subject’s unmodified, natural behavior. Observational methods include several types of examination. Unobtrusive observation is one in which the researcher does not directly participate in the activities that are being observed. Unobtrusive measures are often used to prevent researcher influence on the subject’s behavior. Participant observation allows the researcher to take part in the activities that are being observed and to gain familiarity with the subject’s experiences. Researchers can become fully involved in the activities being observed or they may observe activities they are involved in themselves.
Adrian Holliday, in “What Counts as Data” (2002), creates a schema of the different types of data that may be collected and the methods in which they are collected. Data may include description of (1) behaviors, in which the researcher describes the subject’s behaviors and verbalizations; (2) events, in which the researcher or the subject involved in the event describes the behaviors observed; (3) institutions, in which the researcher describes the rules, regulations, or rituals of the institution; (4) appearance, in which the researcher describes how the environment or the subjects in the environment appear; (5) research events, in which the researcher describes what subjects say or how they behave in the research setting, such as those occurring in interviews; and (6) settings, in which the researcher describes what is actually occurring in a particular setting. These types of data can be obtained through observation notes, research diaries, photographs, or video recordings.
The use of observation as a method of research is valuable for several reasons. Observation allows the investigator to study human behavior as it naturally occurs, with or without the researcher’s influence on the behavior. In other words, human activity is observed without the filtering effects of the subject’s interpretation of their interactions.
Interviewing involves direct interaction between the investigator and the research subject. The investigator speaks directly with the subject asking questions related to a specific topical area. Interviews are generally recorded utilizing audiotapes, videotapes, or written notations. Interviewing may take the form of structured or semistructured interviews.
Andrea Fonatna and James H. Frey, in “The Interview: From Structured Questions to Negotiated Text” (2000), note that structured interviewing allows the researcher to ask targeted questions of the research subjects. This form of interviewing consists of a prepared series of questions asked of all subjects. The subjects’ responses are limited, leaving little room for variation among the answers. The questions are directed at obtaining answers to specific topics of interest for the researcher. Semi-structured interviewing, by contrast, allows for more freedom of discussion with the subject and aims for a greater understanding of the subject. Questions are prepared to prompt topical areas of dialogue. The goal of these questions is to allow the subject to expand upon the question and reveal information that cannot be achieved with a structured interview.
Interviews have several benefits to research. They allow the researcher to discover the meanings of experience that subjects create in relation to the research topics. Interviews provide richer understandings of human behavior by providing real-life examples.
METHODS OF ANALYSIS
Document analysis and archival document analysis are two additional methods utilized in qualitative analysis. Document analysis entails the study of photographs, diaries, newspapers, government documents, books, memorandums, and other written documents. Archival document analysis involves the study of historical documents, including historical examples of those listed above. Importance is placed on analysis of original documents, as opposed to photocopies or other reproductions. While this method allows access to difficult study subjects such as historical figures or societal elites, the study of documents does not allow the investigator to speak to the individual who has created the document. This may lead the investigator to theorize the meanings of the documents and the motivations underlying their creation. These types of studies require the researcher to hypothesize the relationship of the document to the social environment of the time.
The analysis of qualitative data includes a variety of methods, including grounded theory, narrative analysis, and computer-based approaches. As Kathy Charmaz notes in “Grounded Theory: Objectivist and Constructivist Methods” (2000), grounded theory involves an ongoing analysis of the data as it is collected. The data are coded as they are collected and categories begin to emerge. With these categories, theories begin to develop related to the data. These then guide further data collection.
Narrative analysis focuses on the narratives, or stories and experiences, of subjects. Through narrative analysis the researcher endeavors to understand the meaning attached to the experiences of the subject. The researcher may also analyze the specific words employed to describe these experiences.
Computer-based analytic approaches look to the burgeoning World Wide Web for new research subjects. Innovative research is being conducted on subjects found in a variety of Internet communities and through a variety of methods, including online chat rooms, online or e-mail based interviews, and document-type analysis of Web sites. These approaches allow the investigator to contact communities of individuals with similar interests and individuals located outside the investigator’s region or around the world.
RESEARCH VALIDITY, RELIABILITY, AND ETHICS
The use of qualitative methods, however, can be challenging in several respects. As Valerie Janesick notes in “The Choreography of Qualitative Research Design” (2000), qualitative researchers have struggled to address issues of validity and reliability. These terms are used primarily in the interpretation of quantitative data, yet qualitative researchers too are called upon to adhere to these standards of research.
For research to be considered valid it must actually measure what it proposes to measure, and the results should reflect the activity being studied. In other words, the data should accurately reflect the topic being studied. The notion of validity as it relates to qualitative methodology differs from its understanding in quantitative methodology. As Janesick contends, validity in qualitative research asks whether the description and the explanation fit. In an effort to achieve qualitative validity, researchers may permit participants to review the data for accuracy or ask a fellow researcher to do so.
For research to be considered reliable it must yield similar results in subsequent tests. Quantitative reliability is achieved when results are found to be consistent in subsequent tests. Unlike qualitative studies, quantitative reliability is determined through the use of mathematical equations to determine reliability, such as the determination of alpha. Qualitative tests are considered reliable when similar findings are achieved in subsequent tests. However, some debate exists as to whether reliability is pertinent in conducting qualitative studies, particularly since qualitative studies target the meanings and interpretations of experience by the subject and may involve investigator bias. One argument made by some researchers, including Caroline Stenbacka, is that, owing to the nature of qualitative studies and their aim at understanding rather than explaining human experience, the reliability of qualitative research is difficult to assess. Other researchers, such as James F. Davis, Robert Hagedorn, Morton B. King, Jerome Kirk and Marc L. Miller argue that a form of reliability is achievable in qualitative research.
Ethical issues are a concern when using qualitative methods. The protection of the research subject is of the highest priority. Ethical concerns include whether or not the subjects should know they are being observed, how investigator involvement may influence the behaviors under study, how the investigator-interviewee relationship may influence research results, and the bias involved in interpreting qualitative data, as well as protection of subjects’ identities.
In particular, the influence of the investigator on responses given by a subject in interview has been an ongoing concern. Subjects may provide answers they believe the interviewer prefers rather than providing their own unique answers. Subjects may construct answers due to a lack of knowledge of the topical area. Interviewers may unintentionally influence the subject’s answers through their body language, facial gestures, or other responses. As well as the information garnered from interviews is information that has been filtered and interpreted by both the subject and the researcher, calling into question the validity or reliability of the data obtained.
SEE ALSO Ethnography; Methodology; Methods, Research (in Sociology)
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Charmaz, Kathy. 2000. Grounded Theory: Objectivist and Constructivist Methods. In Handbook of Qualitative Research. 2nd ed. Eds. Norman K. Denzin and Yvonna S. Lincoln, 509–535. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Crittenden, Kathleen S., and Richard J. Hill. 1971. Coding Reliability and Validity of Interview Data. American Sociological Review 36 (6): 1073–1080.
Davis, F. James, and Robert Hagedorn. 1954. Testing the Reliability of Systematic Field Observations. American Sociological Review 19 (3): 345–348.
Ellis, Carolyn, and Arthur P. Bochner. 2000. Autoethnography, Personal Narrative, Reflexivity: Researcher as Subject. In Handbook of Qualitative Research. 2nd ed. Eds. Norman K. Denzin and Yvonna S. Lincoln, 751. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Fonatna, Andrea, and James H. Frey. 2000. The Interview: From Structured Questions to Negotiated Text. In Handbook of Qualitative Research. 2nd ed. Eds. Norman K. Denzin and Yvonna S. Lincoln, 649–651. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Holliday, Adrian. 2002. What Counts as Data? In Doing and Writing Qualitative Research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Janesick, Valerie. 2000. The Choreography of Qualitative Research Design. In Handbook of Qualitative Research. 2nd ed. Eds. Norman K. Denzin and Yvonna S. Lincoln, 379–399. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
King, Morton B., Jr. 1944. Reliability of the Idea-Centered Question in Interview Schedules. American Sociological Review 9 (1): 57–64.
Kirk, Jerome, and Marc L. Miller. 1986. Reliability and Validity in Qualitative Research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
Stenbacka, Caroline. 2001. Qualitative Research Requires Quality Concepts of Its Own. Management Decision 39 (7): 551–556.
Gabriela Guazzo
Qualitative Methods
Qualitative methods
Research methods that emphasize detailed, personal descriptions of phenomena.
Research psychologists can collect two kinds of information: quantitative data and qualitative data. Quantitative data are often represented numerically in the form of means, percentages, or frequency counts. Such data are often referred to as "measurement" data, referring to the fact that we often like to measure the amount or extent of some behavior, trait, or disposition. For example, shyness , test anxiety , and depression can all be appraised by means of paper-and-pencil tests which yield numerical scores representing the extent of shyness, anxiety, etc. that resides in the individual taking the test. A psychologist interested in the relationship between test anxiety and grade point average would collect the appropriate quantitative information on each of these two variables and conduct statistical tests that would reveal the strength (or absence) of the relationship.
The term "qualitative research methods" refers to a variety of ways of collecting information that is less amenable to quantification and statistical manipulation. Qualitative methods differ from quantitative methods largely because their ultimate purpose is different. The goal of qualitative research is to arrive at some general, overall appreciation of a phenomenon—highlighting interesting aspects and perhaps generating specific hypotheses. In contrast, quantitative research is typically designed to test relatively specific predictions. Qualitative research thus provides an initial description of a phenomenon, whereas quantitative research aims to investigate its various details. Some examples of qualitative methods include focus groups, surveys, naturalistic observations, interviews, content analyses of archival material, and case studies. What these approaches share is an emphasis on revealing some general pattern by observing a few particular cases.
Focus groups are commonly used by marketing or advertising agencies to derive information about people's reactions to a particular product or event. A small number of people, often fewer than 10, are asked their opinions. A focus group engaged by the marketing department of a breakfast cereal company, for example, might be asked how appealing the cereal looks, whether the box would make them consider buying it, and how agreeable the cereal's texture and taste were. A facilitator would encourage the participants to share their opinions and reactions in the context of a group discussion. The session would be taped and transcribed. Researchers would then use the information to make their product more appealing. Naturalistic observations involve studying individuals in their natural environments. One common variant consists of participant observation research in which the researcher, in order to understand it, becomes part of a particular group. George Kirkham was a criminologist who took a year off from his university position to work as a police patrolman. He then wrote about the changes in his attitudes and values that occurred when he worked in high-crime neighborhood.
There are several drawbacks to qualitative methods of inquiry. Firstly, the results are always subject to personal biases. A person who is interviewed, for example, is stating their version of the truth. Personal perspectives invariably affect what the individual believes and understands. Similarly, the results reported by the researcher conducting a naturalistic observation will be tainted by that researcher's individual interpretation of the events. Further, while case studies are rich sources of information about individuals, it is risky to assume that the information can be generalized to the rest of the population. Moreover, analyzing the data from qualitative research can be difficult, since open-ended questions and naturalistic observation leave room for so much variability between individuals that comparisons are difficult. Finally, although it may be tempting for researchers to infer cause and effect relationships from the results of naturalistic observations, interviews, archival data and case studies, this would be irresponsible. Qualitative methods rarely attempt to control any of the factors that affect situations, so although one factor may appear to have caused an event, its influence cannot be confirmed without conducting more precise investigations. There is thus a tradeoff between flexibility and precision.
The advantages of doing qualitative research are numerous. One of the most important of these is that the flexibility of qualitative data collection methods can provide researchers access to individuals who would be unable or unwilling to respond in more structured formats. For this reason, much research on children is qualitative. Naturalistic observations of children are sometimes undertaken to assess social dynamics. For example, covert videotaping of elementary school playgrounds has revealed that bullying and aggression are far more common than most teachers and parents realize, and that bullying is not uncommon among girls. Similarly, comparative psychologists learn a lot about the social, behavioral, and cognitive abilities of animals by studying them in their natural habitats. A further advantage of this type of research is that the validity of the results is not jeopardized by the laboratory environment . An animal or child may not act the way they usually would in their natural surroundings if they are studied in a laboratory.
Another important application of qualitative research is in the study of new areas of interest, or topics about which not very much is known. Qualitative research usually yields a lot of information. In contrast with quantitative research, the information gathered by qualitative researchers is usually broadly focused. This means that qualitative methods can yield information about the major factors at play, highlighting areas that might warrant more in-depth quantitative study. Although many researchers believe quantitative methods to be superior to qualitative methods, the two are probably best seen as complementary. Qualitative research can suggest what should be measured and in what way, while controlled quantitative studies may be the most accurate way of doing the actual measuring.
Timothy E. Moore
Further Reading
Murray, J. "Qualitative methods." International Review of Psychiatry, vol 10, no. 4 (1998): 312-316.
Marecek, J. "Qualitative methods and social psychology." Journal of Social Issues vol 53, no. 4 (1997): 631-644.