Amnesia, Functional
AMNESIA, FUNCTIONAL
Analyses of learning and memory increasingly attempt to take account of clinical and experimental research on victims of amnesia. Most of this literature has focused on pathologies of memory associated with demonstrable brain lesions or the administration of centrally acting drugs. The functional amnesias are a collection of memory disorders instigated by processes that do not result in damage or injury to the brain but that do engender a marked increase in forgetting.
Amnesia in the Dissociative Disorders
One major category of functional amnesia occurs within the context of diagnosable psychopathology, especially the dramatic "Dissociative Disorders" listed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) (4th edition) of the American Psychiatric Association. In current diagnostic nosology, this category includes a wide variety of syndromes whose common core is an alteration in consciousness affecting memory and identity.
In dissociative amnesia (also known as psychogenic amnesia, limited amnesia), the patient suffers a loss of autobiographical memory for specific past experiences. It sometimes occurs in cases of violent crime (interestingly, affecting either victims or perpetrators), war neurosis, and other types of posttraumatic stress disorder. Unfortunately, many reports of dissociative amnesia are anecdotal and lack independent corroboration of the purported instigating event. Moreover, it is not possible to reliably distinguish genuine cases of psychogenic amnesia from simulated cases.
In dissociative fugue (psychogenic fugue, functional retrograde amnesia), the amnesia covers the whole of the individual's past life and his or her personal identity; there may also be physical relocation (which gives the syndrome its name). The condition may go unnoticed until the patients are asked personal questions that they cannot satisfactorily answer. Recovery typically begins with the patient's recognition of loss of identity. Recovery of identity and memory per se may occur spontaneously or in response to the appearance of a relative or other salient cue. When the fugue is resolved, the patient is typically left with a limited amnesia covering the period of the fugue.
In dissociative identity disorder (multiple-personality disorder), a single individual appears to manifest two or more distinct identities, each alternating in control over conscious experience, thought, and action. Before World War II, the typical case involved only two or three such "ego states"; more recent cases have tended to present more alter egos, leading some to speculate that iatrogenic and sociocultural factors may account for much of the multiple-personality epidemic of the 1980s. In genuine cases, the personalities are separated by an amnesic barrier. The dissociation may be symmetrical, in which each ego state is ignorant of the other(s) or, more commonly, asymmetrical, in which case an ego state may be aware of some of its counterparts but ignorant of others.
In depersonalization the person believes that he or she has changed in some way or is somehow unreal; in derealization the same beliefs are held about one's surroundings. Because these beliefs are objectively inappropriate, these experiences can be construed as disorders of memory: the person fails to recognize some object, self, or situation with which he or she is objectively quite familiar. Episodes of depersonalization and derealization frequently occur in response to stress and in association with anxiety disorders; they may also be induced by psychedelic drugs and occur spontaneously in a substantial proportion of the normal population.
Although dissociative disorders have been of interest at least since the time of Freud and Janet, they rarely have been studied with controlled experimental procedures. For example, little is known about psychogenic amnesia beyond anecdotes. A few cases of fugue and multiple personality have been studied in the laboratory, but we have no idea how representative they are. Nevertheless, the available evidence suggests a pattern of selective memory deficit in some respects resembles that observed in organic amnesia. Thus, psychogenic fugue impairs memory for past experiences and other aspects of self-knowledge but leaves the patient's repertoire of impersonal procedural and semantic knowledge largely intact. Dissociative identity disorder displays a similar pattern.
Nonpathological Amnesias
In other forms of functional amnesia, dramatic forgetting occurs in the ordinary course of everyday living, albeit with no implication of pathology. For example, people commonly fail to remember their dreams and other events of the night's sleep; attempts to demonstrate sleep learning have been almost uniformly unsuccessful. Theoretical accounts of this memory deficit usually revolve around encoding factors. For example, one hypothesis holds that sleep inhibits the higher cortical centers that support perceptual processing.
Another example of nonpathological functional amnesia is the general paucity of memory for infancy and childhood. As with sleep, most theoretical accounts of this developmental amnesia focus on encoding factors. For example, infantile amnesia (covering the first two years of life) may reflect the child's relative inability to encode symbolic and especially linguistic representations of events; even older children may lack the information-processing capacity to encode retrievable memories.
A dramatic form of forgetting known as posthypnotic amnesia occurs in some hypnotized subjects. In some respects, posthypnotic amnesia may serve as a laboratory analogue of the dissociative amnesias seen in the clinic.
Explicit and Implicit Memory
While the functional amnesias by definition impair explicit memory, some anecdotal and experimental evidence suggests that the amnesia may spare implicit memory, or the unconscious influence of past events on subsequent experience, thought, or action. In dissociative identity disorder, for example, both procedural learning and priming effects may transfer between personalities, so that one alter ego is influenced by the experiences of another even though the amnesic barrier prevents conscious recollection. The situation is complicated, however, because not all forms of implicit memory are equally spared. There has been no experimental corroboration of clinical claims that special procedures such as hypnosis and barbiturate narcosis can promote conscious access to the "lost" memories.
Among nonpathological amnesias, the dissociation between explicit and implicit memory is especially well documented in posthypnotic amnesia. Preverbal infants can show long-term retention of new learning in a manner that suggests implicit memory. However, sleep-learning procedures do not appear to leave any traces, even in implicit memory.
Trauma, Repression, and Dissociation
The lack of reliable evidence of brain damage has fostered a tendency to account for functional amnesias in purely psychological terms. Since the nineteenth century, repression and dissociation have been the favored explanations. Repression, as defined by Freud, is the motivated forgetting of material (typically, relating to sexual or aggressive ideas and impulses) that conflicts with physical reality or social sanctions. Dissociation, as discussed by Janet and Prince, is a more adventitious "splitting off" from awareness of a set of percepts, memories, thoughts, or feelings. While Freud argued that repressed contents could be known only by inference (because they were expressed only symbolically, as in dream contents), Janet argued that dissociated contents could be recovered directly, by hypnosis and other means. Given the pervasive influence of Freudian psychoanalysis in twentieth-century discourse, the repression thesis long held sway. There has been a subsequent revival of the concept of dissociation, as indicated by adoption of "Dissociative Disorder" as a category in the DSM. An eclectic combination of Freudian and Jungian theories formed the basis of clinical theories about trauma and memory that emerged in the late twentieth century. These theories, in turn, promoted the 1980s revival of therapeutic strategies based on the recovery and "working through" of traumatic memories.
Paradoxically, the idea that trauma plays a role in amnesia, while a salient aspect of clinical folklore, is not supported by solid empirical evidence. Animal studies indicate that high levels of emotional arousal stimulate the release of stress hormones that activate the amygdala, leading to enhanced memory. A review of more than sixty longitudinal studies of documented trauma survivors yielded not a single instance of amnesia that could not be accounted for by nontraumatic infantile and childhood amnesia or "organic" factors such as intoxication, anoxia, or head injury. Many clinical studies alleging "repression" of trauma also fail to rule out such factors, especially in cases of infantile and childhood amnesia that occur independently of trauma. Others fail to confirm that the ostensibly traumatic event even occurred or misinterpret subjects' failure to disclose their trauma as a failure to remember it.
There are very few corroborated reports of the therapeutic recovery of traumatically lost memories. Most clinicians apparently assume their patients' memories are valid or refrain from challenging them in order to protect the "therapeutic alliance." The techniques of "memory work" used therapeutically to help patients "recover" traumatic memories also increase the risk of memory distortion and confabulation. There is no evidence that either hypnosis or sedation by barbiturate drugs facilitates the recovery of valid traumatic memories. Whenever "recovered" memories are taken as evidence, whether in the clinic or the courtroom, it is critical that they be accompanied by independent, objective, corroborative evidence.
Neuropsychology of Functional Amnesia
Beginning with the discovery of the syphilis spirochete, a major goal of psychiatry has been to transfer syndromes from the functional to the organic category as their neural bases are revealed. It seems likely that neuroscientific research will pinpoint such causal relationships for the dissociative amnesias as well. For example, a recent PET study of dissociative fugue found no sign of the activity in the right posterior frontal and anterior temporal lobes that usually accompanies recollection of emotionally salient personal experiences but did find activation in the left frontal and temporal regions. Although functional may eventually prove to be a misnomer, for now the term delineates a class of amnesias in which psychological processes rather than brain insult, injury, or disease are the immediate causes of the memory failure.
See also:AMNESIA, INFANTILE; AMNESIA, ORGANIC; AMNESIA, TRANSIENT GLOBAL; HYPNOSIS AND MEMORY
Bibliography
Bootzin, R. R., Kihlstrom, J. F., and Schacter, D. L. (1990). Sleep and cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Eich, E., Macaulay, D., Lowewenstein, R. J., and Dihle, P. H. (1996). Memory, amnesia, and dissociative identity disorder. Psychological Science 8, 417-422.
Ellman, S. J., and Antrobus, J. S. (1991). The mind in sleep: Psychology and psychophysiology. New York: Wiley.
Howe, M. L. (2000). The fate of early memories: Developmental science and the retention of childhood experiences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Kapur, N. (1999). Syndromes of retrograde amnesia: A conceptual and empirical synthesis. Psychological Bulletin 125, 800-825.
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1994). One hundred years of hysteria, Dissociation: Clinical and theoretical perspectives. New York: The Guilford Press.
—— (1996). The trauma-memory argument and recovered memory therapy. In K. Pezdek and W. P. Banks, eds., The recovered memory/false memory debate. San Diego: Academic Press.
—— (1997). Suffering from reminiscences: Exhumed memory, implicit memory, and the return of the repressed. In M. A. Conway, ed., Recovered memories and false memories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
—— (2001). Dissociative disorders. In P. B. Sutker and H. E. Adams, eds., Comprehensive handbook of psychopathology. New York: Plenum.
Kihlstrom, J. F., and Barnhardt, T. M. (1993). The self-regulation of memory: For better and for worse, with and without hypnosis, In D. M. Wegner and J. W. Pennebaker, eds., Handbook of mental control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kihlstrom, J. F., and Schacter, D. L. (2000). Functional amnesia. In F. Boller and J. Grafman, eds., Handbook of neuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Kopelman, M. D. (1995). The assessment of psychogenic amnesia. In A. D. Baddeley, B. A. Wilson, and F. N. Watts, eds., Handbook of memory disorders. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
—— (1997). Anomalies of autobiographical memory: Retrograde amnesia, confabulation, delusional memory, psychogenic amnesia, and false memories. In J. D. Read and D. S. Lindsay, eds., Recollections of trauma: Scientific evidence and clinical practice. New York: Plenum.
Markowitsch, H. J. (1999). Functional neuroimaging correlates of functional amnesia. Memory 7, 561-583.
McNally, R. J. (2002). Remembering Trauma., Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Piper, A. (1993). "Truth serum" and "recovered memories" of sexual abuse: A review of the evidence. Journal of Psychiatry and the Law 21, 447-471.
Piper, A., Pope, H. G., and Borowiecki, B. S. (2000). Custer's last stand: Brown, Schefflin, and Whtfield's latest attempt to salvage "dissociative amnesia." Journal of Psychiatry and Law 28 149-213.
Pope, H. G., Oliva, P. S., and Hudson, J. I. (2000). Repressed memories: B. Scientific status. In D. L. Faigman, D. H. Kaye, M. J. Saks, and J. Sanders, eds., Modern scientific evidence: The law and science of expert testimony. St. Paul, MN: West Publisher.
Reed, G. (1988). The psychology of anomalous experience. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus.
Schacter, D. L. (1986). Amnesia and crime: How much do we really know? American Psychologist 41, 286-295.
—— (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 13, 501-518.
—— (2001). The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Shobe, K. K., and Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Interrogative suggestibility and "memory work." In M. L. Eisen, J. Quas, and G. S. Goodman, eds., Memory and suggestibility in the forensic interview. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Sullivan, M. D. (1990). Organic or functional? Why psychiatry needs a philosophy of mind. Psychiatric Annals 20, 271-277.
John F.Kihlstrom
Daniel L.Schacter